Sunday, July 28, 2013

Modified Plan

Dear Oogy,
As we approach the end of July, I feel the need to give you an update on my project for this month. It has taken an entire month's worth of swatches and contemplation for me to reach the conclusion that my original plan for reverse engineering is not sustainable.  Beginning with this photo:
From the Territory Ahead Fall 2012 catalog
I planned to use this yarn:
and these patterns.
You can see that the blue, green, and brown yarns do not match the picture, and the essence of the sweater was getting lost.  The sweater is cool because it's a bit understated, and my rendition of it was becoming rather overstated.  So, my new plan is to use the dark brown yarn as a contrast color and to PURCHASE a lighter color in the same yarn (Berroco's Ultra Alpaca) for the main color.  I was happy with the woven herringbone pattern and I really like the cable pattern, shown above in the green swatch on the left.  Those, and some stockinette and reverse stockinette with limited contrast color should capture the original intent.

Have you settled on plans for your reverse engineering project?
Love,
Neuro

Wednesday, July 24, 2013

Weekly Brain Update: practice makes perfect?


I thought about calling it Weakly Brain Update, as I found it difficult to relate any of the articles in the July 17 issue of the Journal of Neuroscience to the craft of knitting.  
July 17 issue of the Journal of Neuroscience: more about the cover can be found here
The article that was the source of the colorful cover of this week’s issue seemed like the only potential candidate: Nelson and colleagues examined the impact of practice testing on human memory in a paired-words task and linked better performance to increased activity in the parietal cortex.  It’s nice to see a brain imaging study that supports what we tell students all the time: one of the best ways to study is to practice taking a test on the material.  I’ll be sure to show my students the cover shot before I give the next exam!  I suppose I could relate this to the ability to memorize pattern repeats, but is that more of a procedural, motor memory?  Are we able to remember the repeats more accurately if we make an attempt to knit without the pattern in front of us, and make mistakes?  Actually, I think that does happen, and will happen a lot now that I have approached the 42-stitch repeat large motif of my current project:
 
I've modified the color combinations, based on issues that revealed themselves in the swatch.
After writing the initial draft of this post yesterday, I paid attention to what I was doing today as I began the 42-stitch repeat.  At first I found myself counting to keep track of my placement in the pattern: 1,3,1,1,1,1,1,3,1,1,3,7… It’s when I stopped counting that I made a mistake and had to go back a few stitches.  That would represent the practice test, and would be when the activity in my parietal cortex is at its peak.  Whether or not that is promoting my ability to remember the pattern, we’ll have to see.
 
I reached row 3 of the 42-stitch pattern before it was necessary to face reality and go to work this morning.

The three patterns line up very well.

Even though it was tough to relate this week’s articles to knitting, I can easily say how amazed I am at what neuroscientists are able to study.  For example, it is possible to switch out an amino acid to ever-so-slightly change the function of a protein so that it interacts differently with neighboring proteins.  Kazi and colleagues examined how channel proteins are structured so that they are able to select what gets across the neuron’s membrane.  Specific parts of the channel proteins form little gates that stick into the channel opening.  If the gate malfunctions, too many or not enough ions will cross the channel, affecting the cell’s ability to process signals from other cells.  Another article was about how variations in the shape of DNA in the prefrontal cortex can be used to predict the severity of symptoms of schizophrenia.  The DNA’s shape dictates how well a gene will get transcribed, and the shape can change depending on a person’s genetic make-up or their experiences.  

The article that I read most closely was about a process known as long-term depression (LTD).  LTD, and its counterpart, long-term potentiation (LTP) are widely recognized as cellular mechanisms for learning.  In conditioning a relationship is established between two events, often a noise and a foot shock such that the rat or mouse will learn to get scared when it hears the noise.  This relationship is represented in the brain as increased sensitivity of neurons in the amygdala (LTP) and is produced by an increase in the number of excitable receptors on the cell.  With more receptors, the cell’s response to another cell’s signal is enhanced, in this case leading to a greater fear response to the noise.  In this week’s article by Clem and Huganir, the opposite relationship between extinction and LTD was examined.  

Most of it was electrophysiology, where they used electrodes to stimulate neurons in a dish and then other electrodes to measure changes in excitability of other neurons in the same dish.  They were able to show that specific patterns of stimulation were able to decrease the number of excitable receptors that were expressed in the cells, but only if the neurons came from a mouse that had experienced extinction, in which the noise was presented repeatedly without the shock, until it no longer was scary.  Somehow that new experience changed the circuitry to be less sensitive, by reducing the number of available excitable receptors.  

Their goal was to uncover the cellular mechanism of extinction with the hope that other researchers would be able to improve the success of exposure-based therapies for post-traumatic stress disorder.  In these therapies a person is guided through sounds and sights related to a trauma, to restructure their memory into something less traumatic.  If the cellular changes that correspond to extinction can be worked out, it might reveal new strategies that would make this approach more successful, and maybe take less time in humans. 

Sunday, July 21, 2013

Fair Isle Won

Dear Oogy,
Well, July is speeding by rather quickly, and I have made very little progress on my reverse engineering project.  I was easily distracted by other potential projects.  As I mentioned in the last brain update post, I was being pulled in the lace and fair isle direction.  As the pictures below attest, fair isle won that battle.
I had fun pulling out the shetland wool stash and some reference materials.  I chose three motifs to work with and unbelievably, they are all repeats that fit perfectly into 336 stitches!
From top to bottom, a 9 row-7 stitch border pattern, a 5 row-6 stitch peerie, and a 17 row-42 stitch XO pattern.
The swatch above is pictured before I washed it.  One of my favorite things about fair isle knitting, and using shetland wool, is that it changes dramatically after it's washed.

The washed swatch and the beginning of the sweater.
I made some minor changes to the border pattern so that the middle portion of the diamonds could be seen.  Brownie is taking his job of holding the pattern and calculator very seriously.

Now that I have this started, I plan to return to July's project, I promise.  How are you making out with that blue-grey lace-weight wool?
Love,
Neuro

Wednesday, July 17, 2013

Weekly Brain Update: decisions, decisions...


In the past two weeks the Brain Update was focused on the prefrontal cortex, which has been characterized as a brain area that integrates information and is sensitive to the emotional intensity of events.  This week we turn our attention to an area of the brain that receives information from the prefrontal cortex and is pivotal in translating that information into action: the dorsomedial striatum.

The striatum’s function is to influence motor behavior.  When you decide (prefrontal cortex) that it’s time for your midmorning coffee jolt, you must convert that decision into moving out of your chair, reaching for your wallet, making your way to the Coffee Procurement Facility, and extracting the stack of money from your wallet in exchange for the magical elixir. Meanwhile, you smell the aroma of the coffee, and the baked goods that go so well with it.  You hear the familiar voices of the baristas and your fellow addicts, and you experience the initial rush of that first sip.  All of these sensory experiences are processed by various areas in the cortex, and then are sent to the striatum.  If the decision to get coffee is followed by the positive outcome of the coffee’s effect, then the context surrounding this process gets linked to the enjoyable experience, and tomorrow your decision will lead you to the same place to get more coffee.  With enough experience, the role of the prefrontal cortex in the decision-making process is minimized, and the striatum takes over.  Habit formed.

You can understand why neuroscientists are interested in how the striatum converts an idea into action. It would be possible to help someone who has a serious addiction, to something like cocaine or heroin or nicotine, if the neural basis of compulsive behavior was understood.  The authors of this week’s featured article also point out that even a minor malfunction in this system can lead to disorganized behavior, like that observed in obsessive-compulsive disorder or schizophrenia. 
This image is from a very cool study in which the researchers traced the path of new neurons in the hippocampus.
In this week’s Journal of Neuroscience, Ferguson, Phillips, Roth, Wess, and Neumaier have published their research entitled Direct-Pathway Striatal Neurons Regulate the Retention of Decision-Making Strategies.  Remember the awesome viral-vector optogenetics technique from last time?  These authors used a very similar approach, but instead of selectively activating a set of neurons with a light, they used a drug that would only affect the neurons with a particular receptor protein inserted into them.  So, the dorsomedial striatum was injected with the viral vector, which was taken up only by cells that make a specific neurotransmitter and are known to be connected to an output of the striatum, the so-called “direct” pathway.  They injected two types of receptors, one that would stimulate the cells and one that would inhibit them.  Once the receptors were expressed on this small population of cells, the drug that activates the receptor, but would have no other effect in the rat, could be injected.  With Ferguson and colleagues’ technique, they could target a specific population of cells and thus show which part of the striatum’s complex circuitry was responsible for changing behavior. 

The researchers examined the effect of this technique on decision making in rats.  They trained the rats to press two levers for food.  Pressing one lever gave the rat a single food pellet.  Not bad, if you’re a hungry rat.  Pressing the other lever, however, led to delivery of four food pellets: even better if you’re a hungry rat.  Thus, one lever was deemed “high-reward” and the other “low-reward” and the rats demonstrated a clear preference for the high-reward option. 

Whether the neurons were stimulated or inhibited during the training, the rats were still able to make the right choice.  But, if they were asked a week later to do the same task, the ones that had their cells inhibited had trouble learning the difference between high and low reward levers, and they didn’t show a strong preference for the high-reward.  Conversely, the rats that had their cells stimulated a week earlier learned the difference more efficiently and showed a greater preference for the high-reward option.  One of the most interesting challenges in understanding addiction is to describe the brain changes that occur when a person develops a habit and permanently alters their need for a drug.  Even after years of abstinence, an addict will report that they crave the drug, especially if they find themselves in a drug-related situation, like the coffee shop or a bar.  Ferguson and her colleagues suggest that the neurons they studied in the dorsomedial striatum are involved in this long-term change in the brain’s response.
Lace-weight cashmere (above) or classic shetland wool?

So which of the following would you choose?

If you have prior experience with cashmere and you enjoy making lace, you might opt for the pretty green yarn, it being your “high-reward” option.  But what if you are currently on a Fair Isle knitting kick?   It depends on your experience.  

I expected to be cured of the fair isle compulsion for awhile once this was done.
Were you especially pleased with your latest Fair Isle project?  Are you in the same context (season, maybe) as when you had such an enjoyable experience making steeks? Any positive past knitting experience might have changed the sensitivity of your dorsomedial striatum, which would influence the choice you make in beginning your next project.
I find the combination of colors so much fun to arrange.

But, this lacy confection is appealing, too.

Monday, July 8, 2013

Weekly Brain Update: Regulating Emotion


Thank you for your comments on last week’s Weekly Brain Update.

The July 3 issue of the Journal of Neuroscience was full of interesting articles and has provided yet another example of how the prefrontal cortex relates to knitting.  I wonder if this will be the case every week?  I did try my best to devise a link with other articles, ones that focused on hyperalgesic priming, enhanced visual cortex activity in people with amygdala damage, epigenetic regulation of novelty-seeking, and monkey eye-hand coordination, but the prefrontal cortex paper won.

In the article entitled Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Encodes Emotional Value, the authors frame their work in terms of neuroeconomics*: decision making with regard to money is dependent on brain-mediated emotional experiences, both negative and positive.  One example that they give is how a nice garden might induce potential home-buyers to spend more on a house than they would otherwise.  There is also evidence that positive emotion increases the likelihood of impulsive spending, a common experience to anyone who never leaves their favorite yarn store empty-handed. 

Regulating positive emotion is also important if you’re trying to limit your food intake.  The sight of a cake, or a bag of chips, or a stack of chocolate chip cookies automatically stimulates the reward areas of the brain, including the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), which computes the value of those items and makes a comparison with one’s eventual goals, such as fitting into last year’s Fall wardrobe.  When making a decision about whether to convert the action of looking to that of eating, research suggests that how enticing the object is perceived and how easy it is to resist relates to one’s ability to regulate emotion.  If you are able to redefine the cake as a mixture of oil, sugar, and flour and to talk yourself out of eating it, you have probably done so because you’ve lowered the value of the cake by decreasing the activity in your vmPFC.
 
Brownie has a space reserved on top of the Pretend Yarn Store
Do you find the mere sight of yarn to be a positive emotional experience?  After years of trying to impose some order on the stash, I finally decided to display it.  Now I can look up from reading/grading/writing emails and get a little dose of reward.  Anyone visiting me looks at the display and wonders about my sanity, not understanding how years of playing with yarn has given it the special power of making me happy to see it.

The research by Winecoff, Clithero, McKell Carter, Bergman, Wang and Huettel is focused on the role of the vmPFC in the perceived subjective value of rewards.  Their study involved asking people to rate the emotional content of a range of images, some being very positive, like a cute puppy, and some being very negative, such as an infected cut [the actual pictures are kept secret so that participants in the studies have not seen them before; these examples are guesses]. They were instructed to either experience the emotional content of the image (the “experience” group) or to attempt to regulate the emotional impact of the image using a cognitive reappraisal strategy, by detaching themselves from it, or viewing it objectively as having no relationship to oneself (the “regulation” group).  After giving the participants ample practice experiencing and regulating emotion, the researchers then rolled the participants into a function MRI machine in order to examine how much the vmPFC was activated.  They expected that there would be a decrease in activity when a person was regulating their reaction to an image that has been deemed by many to induce positive emotion.  They also proposed that there would be an increase in vmPFC activity in the regulation group when they viewed sad images, which has been demonstrated in other studies.  They showed the expected results, providing some clarity regarding the role of the vmPFC.  It’s not merely a brain area that is responsible for regulating emotion, but also is capable of tracking the degree to which an event or image is positive or negative.      
 
https://plus.google.com/+websyarn/posts
Does this image increase the activity of your vmPFC?
We have all had the experience of buying yarn that we didn’t need, but it’s difficult to resist (high activity in vmPFC) when the yarn is arranged in a rainbow of colors, neatly stacked in sweater-sized piles in the yarn shop.  I think that this is especially problematic for me when I go to WEBS—I have such a positive emotional attachment to that place that my vmPFC must be very active when I walk through the door, and then it goes into overdrive as I approach the warehouse in back [see this visitor’s picture], with the stacks of yarn, neatly arranged in bags.   If I were in the “experience” group in Winecoff et al.’s study, I would be instructed to revel in the joy that is the WEBS warehouse.  If I was in the “regulation” group, I would be instructed to consider the yarn as anyone else might see it, as something that does not relate to my interests, or as mere piles of cotton, silk, wool, and alpaca that are not important and have no specific purpose.  I’m not sure the attempt to regulate my emotion would be strong enough to overcome the impact of WEBS on my wallet, but there might be some barely-detectable decrease in the activity in my vmPFC.

One idea that this research makes me consider is how can we strengthen the function of the vmPFC so that it is easier to regulate emotion?  Stress can interfere with PFC function in general, so it makes sense that it is more difficult to make good choices when stress levels are high.  I won’t be surprised to learn that regular exercise promotes the function of the vmPFC.  I’ll look into it!

*neuroeconomics is a very interesting subfield of neuroscience.  See the following links for more info: Society for Neuroeconomics conference,  NYU Center for Neuroeconomics, Stanford Neuroeconomics.

Saturday, July 6, 2013

Engineering update

I hope your July is going as well as mine!
Oogy-spun teal (emerald?) alpaca yarn is so nice.

I dyed this wool yarn several years ago.  It came from a farm nearby.

I peeked ahead in July and am looking forward to some of the lace patterns I saw mid-month...