In an effort to bring more neuroscience to the Knit with
your Brain blog, I begin this week with a new feature: Weekly Brain Update. Each week the Society
for Neuroscience publishes an issue of the Journal of Neuroscience. This is a premiere journal with a broad range
of neuroscience articles, including ones that are focused on cellular and
molecular mechanisms, as well as ones that describe the neurocircuitry of
complex human behaviors. My plan is to
choose an article from each issue and describe to you how the research relates
to knitting. My real goal is to motivate
myself to read the journal every week; a secondary benefit will be to increase
the neuroscience content of this blog.
A 16-stitch repeat that requires a hard working memory |
One article from the June 26 issue has
especially caught my attention. It is
about working memory and the brain mechanisms that allow us to hold information
in our minds long enough to make links between events. As a knitter, you might think about the
process by which you learned the knit stitch, the sequence of events that had
to happen in a particular order to form one stitch after another. By the time you get to the step of
transferring the completed stitch from the left to right needle, you needed to
remember what was the first step from several seconds before. After some practice, the sequence gets
unified, and you don’t even think of the knit stitch as a series of steps
anymore. Working memory is the process
of keeping the relevant information at the conscious level for a short time (it
is also called short-term memory by some) so that long-lasting memories can be
established by linking events together.
The brain area that many believe makes working memory
possible is the medial portion of the prefrontal cortex. It resides in the front of the brain: if you hold your pointer fingers alongside
your nose, right up against your face pointing straight up, you’ll be pointing
to the medial prefrontal cortex. Someone
who has trouble staying focused on a project might be experiencing some
disruption in this brain area, which has been proposed as a mechanism for
attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Stress can disrupt this area, leading to a range of consequences that
occur when the stress is severe or chronic.
So you can see why it is interesting to find an article
about the mechanism by which the prefrontal cortex contributes to working
memory. What turns out to be even more
interesting about this article is that the researchers have used the most
amazing new neuroscience tool: optogenetics!
Before this technique was developed the only way to manipulate a circuit
in the brain was to inhibit or excite neurons near the circuit, or to alter the
function of a single neuron, which provided very limited information about the
whole circuit. These old methods were
also very slow compared to the processing speed of our amazing neurons. With optogenetics it is possible to alter a very
specific population of neurons for a very short time without affecting their
normal function. The neurons are altered
by injecting a virus with a DNA sequence for a light-sensitive protein into a
brain area. Only a specific population of
neurons will take up the DNA, depending on how it is tagged with a molecule
that only that population of neurons uses (this is where my understanding of
optogenetics is weakest). Later, when
the researchers want to alter the function of those neurons, they shine a laser
nearby, which causes a reaction in the light-sensitive protein and either
excites or shuts down those neurons. If
you want to know more about this technique, I highly recommend the TED
talk by Ed Boyden: A Light Switch for Neurons. I don’t think Dr. Boyden is at all confused
about optogenetics!
Now, back to this week’s article. Researchers at the University of
Wisconsin-Milwaukee examined the role of the medial prefrontal cortex in
forming a memory of an unpleasant event in rats. Rats show overt signs of fear that we can see
easily, in the form of freezing. The rat
remains very still for a period of seconds, sometimes minutes, allowing it to
avoid detection in dangerous situations.
The researchers can easily control how dangerous a situation is to the
rat, by pairing a sound with a brief foot shock. The shock doesn’t hurt the
rat, but it is noticeable and not pleasant, so the rat pays close attention to
the context and anything unique in the context (like the sound) in which this
annoying event has occurred. The greater
the number of pairings between a noticeable sound and a foot shock, the more
dangerous the sound will seem to the rat.
In that context or when hearing that sound, to the extent that the rat
remembers, it will freeze for a period of time, depending on the magnitude of
the perceived danger.
For the rat to benefit from this ability to sense when
dangerous events are likely, it would need to link specific aspects of the
situation that occurred close in time to the unpleasant event. This can be examined by researchers who let
some time elapse between the presentation of the sound and the foot shock. This is a challenge for the brain! Link an event that happened before to one
that is happening later, when the neural trace of the early event would be
gone. Enter the medial prefrontal
cortex, which is thought to somehow keep the information going when its source
is no longer providing it. Gilmartin,
Miyawaki, Helmstetter, and Diba (2013, J. Neurosci, 33(26): 10910-10914) have
provided evidence that the medial prefrontal cortex actually does this
job. They injected the medial prefrontal
cortex of rats (which were anesthetized and positioned in a special frame that
allowed the researchers to target the brain area accurately) with a virus that
would deliver ArchT to the neurons there.
Then they implanted a tiny laser fiber just above where this
light-sensitive protein was delivered.
They waited about 2 weeks for the rats to recover from surgery and when
the light fibers were turned on, the neurons nearby shut down. Their experiment was to turn off the neurons
during the time that elapsed between the sound and foot shock, essentially
removing the ability of the medial prefrontal cortex to keeo the trace in
working memory. They had a bunch of
control groups in which the light was on at different times. Then on the next day the rats were tested for
the strength of their memory. The rats
that had the full function of their prefrontal cortex remembered both the
context and the sound as scary. However,
if the prefrontal cortex was taken offline during the interval between the
sound and the shock, the rats did not remember that the sound was scary. This effect was very specific to the link
between the sound and foot shock; the rats still showed fear in the context by
itself. The optogenetic technique
allowed the researchers to turn off the brain area for a very short time, a mere 20 seconds, which provided the evidence
that was lacking to give the prefrontal cortex full credit for its working
memory function.
This would not be fun to do if we had to constantly consult the pattern. Luckily our working memory keeps track of where we are in the pattern as each repeat is completed. |
What would happen to our ability to learn the knit stitch if
the medial prefrontal cortex was offline between “insert right needle into
front loop of stitch on left needle” and “transfer newly formed stitch to right
needle”? We’d have to keep looking up
the intermediate steps of “wrap yarn around the back of the right needle” and
“draw loop on right needle through the loop on left needle” and the sequence
would not get unified into a single event, the knit stitch. We could tax the medial prefrontal cortex
even more by trying to learn a lace or fair isle pattern repeat. Next time you accomplish those amazing feats,
consider thanking your medial prefrontal cortex!
4 comments:
Do you remember what that amazing swatch is?
Braaaiiiinnnnnnsssss.... Now we can have awesome knitting content AND learn neuroscience - an excellent combo!!
You do a great job of explaining the science in an easily understandable way. Plus, I love the analogy between knitting and prefrontal activity. Thanks!
The swatch is part of the fair isle vest I just finished--it is pattern #159 in Mucklestone's 200 Fair Isle Motifs.
Post a Comment